Non-Word Repetition in Arabic-speaking children with and without Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD): A closer look into accuracy and error patterns

Introduction

 

Non-Word Repetition (NWR) tasks are widely regarded as effective tools for assessing phonological skills. They involve asking children to repeat non-words (words that do not exist in a language), and their ability to do so helps researchers and clinicians assess their phonological memory and language processing skills.

 

In this study, researchers focused on Palestinian Arabic, a vernacular spoken by children in the Muthallath region of Israel. NWR tasks help detect language impairments, such as difficulties in remembering or reproducing sounds, particularly in children with ASD who may display a wide range of language abilities. The study involved a carefully designed NWR task that assessed how well children could repeat non-words of different syllable lengths (ranging from 1 to 4 syllables) and phonological complexity (whether the non-word included consonant clusters or not).

 

Participants and Study Setup

 

A total of 142 Palestinian-Arabic-speaking children, aged 5 to 11, participated in the study. The sample included 75 children with typical language development (TLD) and 67 children diagnosed with ASD. All the children were native speakers of Palestinian Arabic and were raised in Arabic-speaking households, ensuring that the language of the NWR task matched their daily speech patterns.

 

Breakdown of Participants:

 

  • Typical Language Development (TLD) Group: 75 children, equally distributed between boys and girls, aged 5 to 11.
  • Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) Group: 67 children, mostly boys, aged 5 to 11. These children were further classified based on their phonological performance into two subgroups: children with ASD but typical phonological skills (ASD+NL) and children with ASD and phonological impairment (ASD+LI).
See also  Intranasal Oxytocin for Patients With Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Comprehensive Meta-Analysis of Preclinical and Clinical Studies

 

Before the experiment, parental consent was obtained, and the children were asked to assent to participate. The NWR task was administered either in person or via Zoom due to COVID-19 restrictions. The researchers ensured that the remote testing conditions did not affect the results by using proper protocols and maintaining consistency across the different methods of testing.

 

Key Findings: Phonological Performance of Children with ASD and TLD

 

The study provided some significant findings regarding the phonological abilities of children with ASD when compared to their typically developing peers. Here’s a breakdown of the results:

 

1. Heterogeneity in ASD Phonological Skills

 

One of the most crucial findings was the variability within the ASD group. Unlike the TLD children, who showed a relatively consistent performance across the NWR task, the performance of children with ASD was more diverse. Specifically, two distinct subgroups emerged:

  • ASD+NL (ASD with normal language skills): 72% of the children with ASD performed similarly to their TLD peers, showing age-appropriate phonological skills.
  • ASD+LI (ASD with language impairment): 28% of the children with ASD exhibited phonological impairments, performing below their age-matched peers in the NWR task.

 

This result underscores the fact that ASD is a spectrum, not just in terms of social and behavioral skills but also in language development. Some children with ASD demonstrate typical phonological development, while others are at greater risk for language impairments.

 

2. Effect of Non-Word Length on Performance

 

Non-word length, as opposed to complexity, played a significant role in how children performed the NWR task. The study found that:

  • Longer non-words (3 or 4 syllables) were more challenging for children across all groups, but the effect was most noticeable in children with ASD+LI. These children struggled more with longer non-words, indicating difficulties in managing phonological memory for extended syllables.
  • Shorter non-words (1 or 2 syllables) were easier for both TLD and ASD+NL children, but children with ASD+LI still showed more errors even in shorter sequences.
See also  Relation between Class I malocclusion and bad oral habits in children with autism in Makassar

 

Interestingly, the study revealed that phonological complexity, such as the presence of consonant clusters, did not significantly affect performance. This suggests that children, even those with phonological impairments, can manage complex phonological structures in shorter sequences but struggle as the non-words get longer.

 

Error Patterns: How Do Children with ASD and TLD Differ?

 

Understanding the types of errors children make in NWR tasks can offer insights into their underlying phonological skills. The study identified several key error patterns across the groups:

 

1. Consonant Substitution: The Most Common Error

 

Consonant substitution was the most frequent error across both the TLD and ASD groups. This means that when children made mistakes, they tended to replace difficult consonants with easier ones. This error pattern indicates that children could generally form phonological representations but struggled with producing more complex sounds.

 

2. Phoneme and Syllable Omissions

 

Children with ASD+LI made more phoneme and syllable omissions compared to their ASD+NL and TLD peers. This is indicative of deeper phonological processing difficulties in children with ASD+LI, who found it harder to form and maintain complete phonological representations, especially for longer words.

 

3. Multiple Errors in ASD+LI

 

Children with ASD+LI often made multiple errors (e.g., combining omissions and substitutions in a

single non-word). This suggests more profound difficulties in phonological processing, indicating that these children were not just struggling with one aspect of phonological production but with multiple aspects simultaneously. This aligns with previous research that shows children with language impairments tend to have more complex patterns of errors.

 

4. Lexicalization Errors

 

Lexicalization refers to when children replace a non-word with a real word they are familiar with. This was a less common error but still present in both ASD+LI and TLD groups. However, it occurred slightly more in the ASD+LI group, further suggesting that these children might rely on familiar linguistic patterns to compensate for their difficulty in processing non-words.

See also  KCNH5 deletion increases autism susceptibility by regulating neuronal growth through Akt/mTOR signaling pathway

 

Implications for Clinicians and Educators

 

This study offers valuable insights for clinicians, educators, and parents working with children with ASD. Here are some key takeaways:

 

1.     Phonological Assessments Are Crucial for ASD Children

 

The study highlights the importance of comprehensive language assessments for children with ASD, particularly using tools like NWR tasks that can uncover subtle phonological impairments. The identification of subgroups (ASD+NL and ASD+LI) is especially important, as it helps in targeting interventions more effectively.

 

2.     Tailored Interventions Based on Phonological Skills

 

For children with ASD+LI, who exhibit phonological impairments, interventions should focus on strengthening phonological memory and reducing phoneme and syllable omissions. Clinicians can design activities that gradually increase the length of words children are asked to repeat, helping them build their phonological skills step by step.

 

3.     Early Language Support is Key

 

Since phonological skills in typical language development are generally acquired by age five, early identification and support are vital. Children with ASD who show signs of phonological impairment can benefit from early language interventions to mitigate the risk of long-term language difficulties.

 

4.     The Role of Non-Verbal IQ

 

The study also found that non-verbal IQ was a significant predictor of NWR performance. This means that children with higher cognitive abilities generally performed better on NWR tasks. Understanding this relationship can help clinicians design more personalized language interventions, taking into account both cognitive and phonological challenges in children with ASD.

 

Conclusion: A Step Forward in Understanding Phonology in ASD

 

This research marks a significant step forward in understanding how Arabic-speaking children with ASD process language, particularly in the area of phonological development. The study’s findings emphasize the heterogeneity of language abilities in children with ASD, with some demonstrating typical phonological skills and others showing marked impairments.

 

For parents and educators, this study reinforces the need for regular phonological assessments and tailored language interventions. By understanding a child’s specific phonological strengths and weaknesses, we can better support their communication and language development, ultimately improving their quality of life.

 

The next steps in this area of research could involve larger sample sizes and further exploration of how different Arabic dialects might affect phonological development in children with ASD. Moreover, incorporating additional language tasks such as phonological awareness activities could provide a more comprehensive picture of linguistic abilities in these children.

 

For now, this study offers a clearer understanding of how children with ASD process language and highlights the critical importance of early intervention in addressing phonological impairments.

 

Source:

https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/pdf/10.1080/02699206.2024.2391904

Leave a Comment